The team members of the Michigan State University M.A.P. Project are very pleased to announce that we have finished our initial data collection for the study! We hope to continue pursuing our goals in developing a better understanding of children's thought processes and how these processes relate to their patterns of brain activity. For those of you who participated in our study, please be on the lookout for another project newsletter later this fall where we will describe the final results!
It has been a wonderful experience working with each and every one of you! So we would like to say thank you to all of the families that participated
over the last year and a half, we couldn't have done it without your
help!
Monday, September 8, 2014
Tuesday, May 6, 2014
Q&A with Judith Danovitch!
1. What inspired you to become a psychologist?
I think I was always interest in cognitive development (although I didn't know it was called that) because I grew up speaking three languages at home, and I often wondered how that was possible and why it was easy for me. In college, I worked in a psychology lab and taught children in different settings and I realized how much I loved working with children and how interesting it would be to try to understand their thinking processes.
2. What is your favorite part about doing research?
My favorite part of research is collecting data and seeing the results. It is so much fun to hear what children have to say, and it is exciting to discover new aspects of how their minds work.
3. What research finding do you think is most important for parents to know?
I think the research on temperament is extremely valuable for parents. Temperament basically means that different children react to the same experience in different ways. So, for example, one child might experience a roller coaster ride as fun while another child finds it terrifying. I think if parents understood this better, they would be more appreciative of their child's individuality and be less likely to compare their child to other children.
4. What goal do you hope to accomplish with your research?
I hope to give parents, educators, and the general public new insights into how children think at different points in their development.
Thursday, April 3, 2014
Positive and Negative Thinkers' Brains
Research conducted by Michigan State University psychologist, Jason Moser, shows that positive and negative thinking may actually be hardwired in the brain. To read more about these findings in the MSU Today, please click here . Dr. Moser also develop the neurophysiological measures used here at the MSU M.A.P. Project!
Friday, March 14, 2014
Picture of the Week!
Welcome to the Jungle Room!
The Jungle Room is located inside the MSU KID lab. All child participant sessions for the MSU MAP Project take place here!
Friday, March 7, 2014
Infants Learn By Doing
We know that our minds control our actions, but do our actions influence our minds? Psychology researchers have recently tackled this question by looking at how physical movements influence learning in infants and young children.
One new study examined how three-month-olds learn about others’ goals based on their own actions. Infants sat for a few minutes and either played with some toys themselves using Velcro “sticky” mittens that helped them move the toys around or they just watched the experimenter play with the toys. Afterwards, infants watched the experimenter pick up one of the two toys until the infant lost interest. The position of the toys was then switched, and infants saw the experimenter do one of two things: either perform the same action as before (the hand reached out and grabbed the toy on the left) but the outcome was different (a different toy was picked up than before), or the experimenter’s hand performed a different action (the hand reached out and grabbed the toy on the right) but the outcome was the same as before (the same toy was picked up as before). The researchers measured how long the infants looked at each event to determine whether they found either one surprising.
Infants who played with the toys themselves looked longer at the event with the different outcome than event with the same outcome. What does this mean? Infants are able to learn about others’ desires by applying what they know about how they perform actions in the world. They learn that their actions can be directed toward specific outcomes, although this learning is still limited. So what’s the takeaway? Passive learning is not very effective learning, therefore it is crucial that children – and even young babies – act on their own world while learning – i.e., active learning – in order to gain the most knowledge.
To read a more details description of this study and its finding, please click here:
Also, to watch a brief video demonstrating the infants’ use of the Velcro mittens, please click here:
One new study examined how three-month-olds learn about others’ goals based on their own actions. Infants sat for a few minutes and either played with some toys themselves using Velcro “sticky” mittens that helped them move the toys around or they just watched the experimenter play with the toys. Afterwards, infants watched the experimenter pick up one of the two toys until the infant lost interest. The position of the toys was then switched, and infants saw the experimenter do one of two things: either perform the same action as before (the hand reached out and grabbed the toy on the left) but the outcome was different (a different toy was picked up than before), or the experimenter’s hand performed a different action (the hand reached out and grabbed the toy on the right) but the outcome was the same as before (the same toy was picked up as before). The researchers measured how long the infants looked at each event to determine whether they found either one surprising.
Infants who played with the toys themselves looked longer at the event with the different outcome than event with the same outcome. What does this mean? Infants are able to learn about others’ desires by applying what they know about how they perform actions in the world. They learn that their actions can be directed toward specific outcomes, although this learning is still limited. So what’s the takeaway? Passive learning is not very effective learning, therefore it is crucial that children – and even young babies – act on their own world while learning – i.e., active learning – in order to gain the most knowledge.
To read a more details description of this study and its finding, please click here:
Also, to watch a brief video demonstrating the infants’ use of the Velcro mittens, please click here:
Friday, February 21, 2014
Combating the Vocabulary Gap Between Rich and Poor Children
Recent research has raised concerns that the vocabulary gap
is widening between children from families of varying socioeconomic status. By
18 months, socioeconomic status differences in language processing skills and
vocabulary are already visible. By the time a child is three-years-old, those
growing up in lower-income families may hear up to 30 million fewer words than
children from higher-income families. What does this mean for these children as
they grow up? By the time a child is ready to attend school, those from
lower-income families have been shown to score two years behind on standardized
language development tests.
What is
causing this gap to emerge as early as infancy? Researchers believe parenting
styles and home learning environments are the two most crucial factors
contributing to income-related gaps in school readiness. Many parents don’t
realize the power they hold to foster learning in their children prior to
beginning formal education. The good news is that if parents are given the
proper knowledge and skills, they can help close this vocabulary gap starting
with their own children.
Toddlers learn vocabulary through
context, so the faster a child recognizes words they already know, the better
they can figure out and learn the following words in the sentence. A parents’
best bet to combating this language and vocabulary gap is to turn off the
television, read to their child, and use more child-directed speech. These
simple techniques have shown greater efficacy in children’s language
processing, therefore increasing a child’s vocabulary. Researchers have created
a simple method for parents and caregivers to use to expand a child’s
vocabulary:
·
Tune In: pay attention to what the child is focused
on or is communicating to you
·
Talk More: use lots of descriptive words to
build their vocabulary
·
Take Turns: engage in conversations with the
child
By utilizing these three simple
methods, parents can help increase their children’s
vocabulary at an early age. High school achievement gaps
between socioeconomic statuses are already visible in kindergarten. Since most
language development and vocabulary gaps are already evident by age two, it is
critical to encourage development early on to prevent a ripple effect
throughout the rest of the child’s life. However, parent education intervention
studies with low-income families have shown very promising results. By using
techniques to foster language development early, parents have the ability to
change their child’s life trajectory.
To read more details about this article and the studies
discussed, please click here.
Sunday, February 9, 2014
Raising Successful Children
Everyone wants to be a great
parent, but what are some of the best ways to raise happy and successful children? The optimal parent is one who is involved, but not too involved, one
who is responsive to their child’s needs and sets high expectations, but respects
their child’s independence. Children of these “authoritative” parents perform
better academically, physically, and socially. By not being overly involved,
your child will develop higher confidence and motivation.
In one study, a researcher gave children
a simple puzzle to solve and only some of the children were told how bright and
capable they were. Children who were not given the praise were more motivated
to tackle more difficult puzzles, showed higher levels of confidence and
greater overall progress in puzzle-solving. These results show that praising
talents and abilities actually decreases a child’s confidence because they risk
losing their status by trying more difficult puzzles. Other studies have shown
similar findings, for example, supporting a child’s independence and limiting parental
involvement results in better academic and emotional outcomes.
Parents of successful and happy
children did not do for the child what the child could do or could almost do
themselves. Children face challenges everyday that they must learn to overcome
on their own without their parent taking over. Parents must understand that
their children need to experience successful failures, or failures that the
child can live with and grow from. Depriving children of challenges also
deprives them from learning the skills they’ll need to survive in life. Not
everything in a child’s life is always going to go according to plan, so
parents must learn to accept this and allow their child to be independent.
Overparenting can be detrimental to
a child’s confidence, motivation, and independence. Children flourish when
their environment is reliable, consistent, available, and noninterfering.
Allowing children to make mistakes can be anxiety-provoking for parents, but
parents must learn to control their anxiety to allow their children to manage
the given situation on their own. Unhappy and unfulfilled parents are more
likely to be prone to overparenting, so make sure your needs are also met in
order to be the very best parent you can be. Happy parenting!
Click here to read a more in-depth article on this topic from the New York Times!
Wednesday, January 22, 2014
Friday, January 17, 2014
Excessive Praise May Actually Be Harmful to Children
Over the years, more and more research has focused on the effects of praise across a variety of situations. Praise has been used as a motivator and it often acts as a reward for children, but recent research raises questions about the effects of excessive praise on children's behavior. Turns out that excessive praise may be detrimental to children with low self-esteem. Adults are twice as likely to use excessive praise towards children with low self-esteem, in comparison to children with high self-esteem. This makes sense because adults often want to give additional praise to the children that seem to need it most, however, this appears to have the opposite effect on children with low self-esteem.
In one recent study, children drew a Van Gogh picture, and were either given: inflated praise (e.g. "You did an incredibly great job!"), non-inflated praise (e.g. "You did a great job!"), or no praise by a professional painter. Then, children were given the choice to draw one of two types of pictures: an easier picture where they wouldn't learn much, or a difficult picture where they would make lots of mistakes but would also learn a lot. Children with low self-esteem who received inflated praise were more likely to choose the easier picture to draw.
So what exactly is inflated praise doing to children with low self-esteem? Praise sends a message about future standards and expectations. When a child with low self-esteem hears excessive, inflated praise, they feel that they must do exceptionally well or else they will disappoint others, therefore they shy away from challenges that could result in failure. So, it seems that, like with many things, the best practice is to praise in moderation - don't overdo it.
To learn more about these studies and what others are saying about them, please visit the following links:
In one recent study, children drew a Van Gogh picture, and were either given: inflated praise (e.g. "You did an incredibly great job!"), non-inflated praise (e.g. "You did a great job!"), or no praise by a professional painter. Then, children were given the choice to draw one of two types of pictures: an easier picture where they wouldn't learn much, or a difficult picture where they would make lots of mistakes but would also learn a lot. Children with low self-esteem who received inflated praise were more likely to choose the easier picture to draw.
So what exactly is inflated praise doing to children with low self-esteem? Praise sends a message about future standards and expectations. When a child with low self-esteem hears excessive, inflated praise, they feel that they must do exceptionally well or else they will disappoint others, therefore they shy away from challenges that could result in failure. So, it seems that, like with many things, the best practice is to praise in moderation - don't overdo it.
To learn more about these studies and what others are saying about them, please visit the following links:
“Stop Heaping Praise
on Your Kids”
“When Praise Hurts:
The Psychology of Gushing”
Friday, January 10, 2014
Understanding Children's Emotions
Understanding the importance of emotions in children is
crucial to understanding how emotions guide their thoughts and behaviors.
Recent research has defined interest as a key emotion that may be the first to
develop in infancy, then later stimulates children’s curiosity, motivating them
to explore the world around them and to learn from their experiences. Parents are encouraged to respond
enthusiastically to their children’s interests, increasing opportunities for
meaningful conversations which builds stronger family relationships.
To read a more detailed article about this concept of understanding children's emotions, click on the following link: http://www.huffingtonpost.com/kenneth-barish-phd/understanding-childrens-e_b_3594455.html .
Saturday, January 4, 2014
Project Update!
The MSU Minds, Attitudes, and Parenting project is pleased to announce that we've reached the midpoint of our study, and have had a great turn out thus far! Over 65 children and their parents have participated in our study over the last six months. Interesting results between children's behaviors, their brain activity, and their parents' attitudes are beginning to emerge. We will be presenting some of these findings at the upcoming meetings of the Association for Psychological Science in San Francisco and at the Intellectual Humility conference in Saint Louis. We're very excited about the data we have collected already and are even more excited to see what the next six months will bring! Thank you to everyone who has already participated in this study and to those that have helped spread the word about our research! It has been a pleasure working with the families and we hope the results of our study will provide helpful information in return in the not too distant future. In the coming year, we hope to begin posting results from our study and, begin providing workshops and events to inform the public about our research. Until then, we are still looking for children and parents to participate over the next six months, so please continue spreading the word about the MSU MAP project. For more details about this research study, feel free to e-mail us at msukidlab@gmail.com or visit our website at www.msumap.org .
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)